FUNCTIONAL ORGANIZATION OF SENSORY AND ADJACENT CORTEX OF THE MONKEY12 [44]

J.G. Dusser de Barenne, H.W. Garol and W.S. McCulloch

In previous papers concerning the functional organization of the sensory cortex(1, 2) the boundaries of that area have been mentioned and it has been stated that strychninization immediately outside of it failed to fire any part of it. So clear was the evidence to this effect in Macaca mulatta that it was used as a criterion of the sensory cortex in the researches on the chimpanzee.(3, 4, 5, 6) The present study deals with the properties and relations of these adjacent regions and with detailed observations on constituent areas of the sensory cortex which had to be precisely determined in order to be certain as to the site of electrodes and strychninizations here employed. These experiments were interspersed among studies of the same regions on the chimpanzee's brain in which it was often possible to keep strychninization confined well within a single physiologically unique band and thus to obtain a clearer conception of what to attempt in the monkey. More precise knowledge of several areas has thus been brought to light so that, aided by occasional histological control, it is now possible to improve on the previous diagram of functional organization within the sensory cortex and to be relatively certain about cytoarchitectonic correlations.

Methods

All experiments were performed upon monkeys (Macaca mulatta and one mangabey) fully anaesthetized with Dial3 (0.45 cc. per kg. body weight, ½ the dose given intraperitoneally, ½ intramuscularly). The animal's head was then secured in a head holder. Strychnine was applied to the cortex by placing thereon pieces of filter paper, 1 to 4 sq. mm., moistened with 3 per cent solution of strychnine colored with toluidine blue. All records of electrical activity were made by means of six channel Grass amplifiers and ink writers. Thirty-six electrodeseach a silver wire ending in a small silver ballwere placed on the cortex. These were arranged in groups of 6 to facilitate switching and the activity of all 36 loci was recorded before and several times after each local strychninization.

The identification of the strychninized area was made by pricking or slitting it with a sharp knife at the time the strychnine paper was removed, fixing the brain in alcohol, sectioning, and staining with toluidine blue. For the certain cytoarchitectonic identification of areas 2s and 19s with Brodmann's 2 and 19 respectively we are indebted to Dr. Gerhardt von Bonin.

Results

Application of strychnine to any gray mass causes large sudden voltages to appear in all places where the axons, or collaterals, of cells in the region strychninized terminate. These sudden voltagescalled strychnine spikesserve, therefore, to reveal the directed functional relation of various cortical areas.(7)

Figure 1 is a new map of the sensory cortex and adjacent regions. It is made on the basis of the previous maps of the sensory cortex and has been extended in conformity with the new findings. The numbers indicate the cytoarchitectonic areas of Brodmann(8) and the Vogts(9) but are simplified in the face region where no distinction could be made on the basis of organization

Figure 1

Figure 1. Macaca mulatta. Diagram of sensory and adjacent cortex and convexity of hemisphere. Areas 6, 4s, 4, 3, 1, 2s, 5 and 7 constitute the sensory cortex. Letters L, A and F indicate the functional subdivisions for leg, arm and face respectively. indicates areas from which suppression of electrical activity of the cortex can be obtained. Of these areas 8s and 19s are immediately adjacent to the sensory cortex but not part of it.

between 6b𝜶 and 6bβ. The subdivisions for leg, arm and face are indicated by the appropriate letters.

Since this work was completed a new finding of interest with respect to the sensory cortex has been obtained in experiments on the occipital lobe in which connection it will be fully reported later. In area A7 immediately below the sulcus interparietalis and sometimes confined to it lies a narrow strip of cortex which is fired from the occipital lobe. To date it is the only part of the sensory cortex fired by strychninization of the cortex anywhere outside the sensory cortex.

Insofar as the results merely confirm previous findings they will not be reiterated except by schematic representation in the conclusion. All results to be considered are of one of two types, namely the occurrence of the large sudden voltages, called strychnine spikes from their appearance in the record, or the transient diminution or disappearance of electrical activity produced by strychninization of restricted areas and called the suppression of electrical activity. (i) Local strychninization of area 9 neither produces strychnine spikes (i.e., fires) nor reduces the electrical activity of (i.e., suppresses) any area of the sensory cortex. Occasionally its strychninization is followed by increase of activity of several cortical areas but this occurs so irregularly and its site is so various that it must depend upon other variables than those controlled in these experiments. Except for this, the only effect of strychninization of area 9 is the firing of area 9. (ii) Local strychninization of area 8s fires area 8s locally and suppresses the electrical activity of the entire hemisphere.(10, 11)

Figure 2

Figure 2. March 2, 1940. Macaca mulatta. Dial. Strychninization of 8s showing suppression first seen in A2 later in L4 then in A4.

The suppression appears first in regions nearest to area 8 and sweeps across the cortex. It usually begins 7 to 12 min. after the application of strychnine, lasts several minutes and requires about 15 to 20 min. to cross the sensory cortex. Figure 2 shows the records of such a suppression. (iii) Nothing new has been discovered with respect to strychninization of area 6 except that its anterior margin has several times been found well forward in the concavity of the sulcus arcuatus. (iv) Strychninization of 4s (the strip of Marion Hines(12, 13)) has twice been accomplished without impinging on area 6 or area 4. (This was done only after experiments on the chimpanzee had shown the simplified picture which one might hope from a pure 4s strychninization.) It fired itself locally and no other area and it produced a suppression like that obtained from area 8. (v) Local strychninization of area 4 so far has yielded no new findings although on the basis of the finding on the chimpanzee(14) one would have expected them. (vi) Local strychninization of area 3 fires itself very strongly and also areas 1, 2 and 5 but weakly. It produces very questionable firing, if any, in area 4 and perhaps (on one occasion) in area 7. (vii) Local strychninization of area 1 fires itself and areas 2, 5 and 7 strongly. It also fires weakly area 4 and possibly area 4s. (viii) Local strychninization of area 2s in the leg- or arm-subdivision fires itself only locally and gives a suppression of the entire cortex like that seen after strychninization of 8s and 4s. In the face area the attempt to strychninize area 2s has usually been unsuccessful and when successful has produced at most a weak suppression. (ix) No new findings have appeared after strychninization of area 5. (x) The same may be said of strychninization of area 7. (xi) Between area 7 and the first temporal sulcus there is sometimes, but not always, demonstrable a narrow triangular areaarea 22local strychninization of which fails to fire or suppress any part of the sensory cortex. (xii) Immediately posterior to the sensory cortex medial to the sulcus interparietalis and either separated from it only by the medial prolongation of area 22 or only by the first temporal sulcus lies area 19s strychninization of which produces a suppression like that obtained from 8s, 4s and 2s.

Two observations concerning the variations of the areas with respect to anatomical landmarks deserve mention.

(i) Area 8s, identified by the elicitation of eye movements and by strychninizations which yield suppression of electrical activity of the cortex, extends from the medial aspect of the hemisphere(11, 15) laterally between areas L6 and 9 to almost the superior end of the medial limb of the sulcus arcuatus. From this point downward it usually passes into the anterior lip of the sulcus to emerge again only for a short distance as it crosses the concavity of the sulcus. However, it has appeared in one animal on the medial lip of the upper ramus, disappeared into the depths to reappear on the surface posterior and lateral to the sulcus in what is normally F6b; whereas, in another animal it crossed the concavity ending anterior to the tip of the inferior ramus.

(ii) Even greater variation occurs at the posterior margin of the sensory cortex above the sulcus interparietalis, where exploration is rendered difficult by a complex of veins in the vicinity of the sulcus parieto-occipitalis. Here precise localization can be obtained in only a small percentage of hemispheres, yet even these few disclose that area 5 sometimes extends into the sulcus parieto-occipitalis, and area 19 appears on the occipital lobe posterior thereto. In extreme cases area 5 stops short and area 19 appears in the parietal cortex and may even be separated from the sulcus parieto-occipitalis by an area a millimeter or two wide exhibiting properties of an area normally found in the occipital lobe. This latter arrangement resembles closely the relation to landmarks most frequently encountered in the chimpanzee.

Discussion

Two aspects of the present findings should be mentioned. The first of these is the existence of the suppressor bands 8s and 19s which bound the sensory cortex, except for area 22 occasionally coming to the surface in front of the first temporal sulcus. As the animals used in this experiment were frequently used also for studying cortico-striatal projections,(10, 16) it is possible to be certain that area 8s, like area 4s, projects to the nucleus caudatus. As it has already been shown that this projection is responsible for suppression from 4s, it seems only reasonable to conclude that it is likewise effective in the case of 8s, particularly in view of the finding that strychninization of the nucleus caudatus can itself produce suppression by blocking thalamo-cortical reverberations. As yet it has not been possible to demonstrate similar projections from areas 2s and 19s.

Since this statement was made Drs. Hugh Garol and Percival Bailey, working in this laboratory, have shown that strychninization of 2s does fire far posteriorly into the horizontal portion of the nucleus caudatus. This finding makes it advisable to expect connections of 19s to still more caudal portions of the nucleus caudatus.

There are, therefore, on the convexity of the hemisphere of the monkey four suppressor bands, two within and two just without the sensory cortex and all four are alike in their failure to fire other portions of the sensory cortex. Thus it resembles the chimpanzee's functional organization.

The second point requiring comment concerns the clarification of the findings in strychninization of 4s and 2s. In the case of 4s, the pure picture differs from the previous mixed (which we had mistaken for pure) merely by the omission of firing elsewhere. In the case of 2s (which we, lacking histological control, had tentatively called 1) the change is more complicated. Reference to the previous diagram of functional organization(1) shows that areas 2 and 5 had no difference as to other regions fired from them, a finding out of line with those for all other areas strychninized. The difference which now appears is that the area we had considered to be 5 failed to fire other parts of 5, whereas 2 did so. It is now clear that both were really area 5, and that within 5 the firing is more restricted in the most posterior portion. In the paper mentioned above all mention of leg or arm 3 was omitted, for it is difficult to strychninize it without touching area 1 and the difference in firing of other areas from 3 and 1 is not clear enough to be absolutely certain of the results. The same may be said of pickup electrodes intended for area 3, i.e., disturbances from 1 may be included. It seems therefore wisest to omit area 3 from the schematic representation of the findings except for the face region. When these modifications are made the new scheme and the new map bear a still more striking resemblance to that of the chimpanzee, at least so far as the leg and arm regions are concerned.

As already stated, the work on the chimpanzee gave clues as to what might be expected in the monkey, for, being larger, the bands of the chimpanzee's cortex were more easily strychninized separately. Lest the work on the chimpanzee be thought to have been unduly influential in producing the similarities noted above it should be emphasized that, in the monkey, repeated careful attempts to distinguish two bands in area 4 and any subdivision into bands of area 5 or 7 have failed completely, except for the more restricted firing in the posterior part of area 5. Whether the failure to find these differentiations is due to their non-existence or to the small size of the area to be subdivided cannot be determined by the procedures here employed. On the other hand, if one considers the threshold to electrical stimulation,

Figure 3

Figure 3. Functional organization of sensory and adjacent cortex of leg- and arm-subdivisions. Solid triangle indicates location of strychnine; Y, the appearance of strychnine spikes; , the suppression of electrical activity. Diagram indicates the maximum results obtained by strychninizations confined to the areas indicated. Double lines indicate sulcus centralis; triple lines, the confines of the sensory cortex.

area 4 of the monkey is divisible into two bands, and, as excitation by strychninization remains generally more nearly confined to its site than does that by electrical stimulation, one would expect that a difference of functional organization of these bands in 4 would certainly be discoverable it if exists. It was hoped that the somewhat larger brain of a large mangabey might have sufficiently larger areas to show separate bands in areas 5 and 7 but it failed so to do. In this manner and in these respects the monkey's brain seems to lack differentiations found in the chimpanzee and here the maps remain significantly dissimilar.

Summary

The accompanying schematization (Fig. 3) indicates only those findings which are well established in the monkey and summarizes the results of the present and previous investigations of the functional organization of the sensory and adjacent cortex.

Footnotes

References

Dusser de Barenne, J. G., and McCulloch, W. S. Functional organization in the sensory cortex of the monkey (Macaca mulatta). J. Neurophysiol., 1938, 1: 69-85.

Dusser de Barenne, J. G., McCulloch, W. S., and Ocawa, T. Functional organization in the face-subdivision of the sensory cortex of the monkey (Macaca mulatta).J. Neurophysiol., 1938,1: 436-441.

Dusser de Barenne, J. G. The sensory cortex of the chimpanzee's brain. Science, 1939, 90: 403-404.

Dusser de Barenne, J. G., Bailey, P., Garol, H. W., and McCulloch, W. S. Sensory cortex of the chimpanzee. Amer. J. Physiol., 1940, 129: 303-304.

Dusser de Barenne, J. G., Bailey, P., Garol, H. W. and McCulloch, W. S. Sensory cortex of chimpanzee. J. Neurophysiol., 1940,3: 469-485.

Dusser de Barenne, J. G., and McCulloch, W. S. The sensory cortex of the chimpanzee. Proc. Soc. exp. Biol., N. Y., 1939, 42: 27-29.

Dusser de Barenne, J. G., and McCulloch, W. S. Physiological delimitation of neurones in the central nervous system. Amer. J. Physiol., 1939, 127: 620-628.

Brodmann, K. Neue Ergebnisse über die vergleichende histologische Lokalisation der Grosshirnrinde mit besonderer Berücksichtigung des Stirnhirns. Anat. Am., 1912, 41 (Suppl.): 157-216.

Vogt, C., and Vogt, O. Allgemeinere Ergebnisse unserer Himforschung. J. Psychol. Neurol., Lpz., 1919, 25: 277-462.

Dusser de Barenne, J. G., Garol, H. W., and McCulloch, W. S. Physiological neuronography of the cortico-striatal connections. Res. Publ. Ass. *nerv. ment. Dis., 1941, 21: (in press).

Garol, H. W. Some observations on suppression of electrical activity of areas 4 and 6. Amer. J. Physiol., 1940,129: 361.

Hines, Marion. The anterior border of the monkey's (Macaca mulatto) motor cortex and the production of spasticity. Amer. J. Physiol., 1936, 116: 78.

Hines, Marion. The motor cortex. Bull. Johns Hopk. Hosp., 1937, 40: 313-336.

Dusser de Barenne, J. G., Garol, H. W., and McCulloch, W. S. Motor cortex of chimpanzee. J. Neurophysiol., 1941, 4: 287-303.

Richter, C. P., and Hines, Marion. Increased spontaneous activity produced in monkeys by brain lesions. Brain, 1938, 61:1-16.

Dusser de Barenne, J. G., and McCulloch, W. S. Sensorimotor cortex, nucleus caudatus and thalamus opticus. J. Neurophysiol., 1938, 1: 364-377.

For further research:

Wordcloud: Activity, Adjacent, Appears, Area, Bands, Barenne, Brain, Caudatus, Certain, Chimpanzee, Confined, Cortex, Dusser, Electrical, Except, Experiments, Face, Failed, Figure, Findings, Fires, Functional, Garol, Hemisphere, Indicates, Leg, Local, Macaca, McCulloch, Medial, Mentioned, Monkey, Mulatta, Neurophysiol, Nucleus, Obtained, Organization, Possible, Posterior, Previous, Produced, Regions, Respect, Results, Sensory, Several, Strychninization, Sulcus, Suppression?

Keywords: Monkey, Area, Sensory, Cortex, Science, Regions, Brain, Areas, Mulatta, Methods

Google Books: http://asclinks.live/uaj0

Google Scholar: http://asclinks.live/fpe5

Jstor: http://asclinks.live/hht3


1 Reprinted from Jo_urnalof NeTyphvsiologa, 1941, 4:324-330.
2 Aided by a grant from the Fluid Research Funds orthe Yale School of Medicine.
3 We wish to thank the Ciba Co. for kindly putting the Dial at our disposal.